Peter Kramer’s Defense of Antidepressants

Cover of Listening to Prozac

“Could drugs that are ingested by one in 10 Americans each year, drugs that have changed the way that mental illness is treated, really be a hoax, a mistake or a concept gone wrong?”  asks Brown University psychiatrist Peter Kramer in this NYTimes.com article. Kramer lambasts the current meme that antidepressants are no better than placebo, especially for mild depression. His major point is that the research on which that conclusion is based was contaminated by a recruitment process that selected many subjects who were not truly depressed. Thus, when followed over time, they got better regardless of whether they were on antidepressants or placebos, obscuring the value of the drugs for truly depressed patients.(For a more sophisticated discussion, in my opinion, of the reasons why there has been a lessening gap between medications and placebos, see this article in Wired by Steve Silberman.)

Kramer is best known for his popular 1993 book, Listening to Prozac, in which he argued that some people feel “better than well” when treated with such medications. In particular, energy, assertiveness and self-confidence can be enhanced even if they were not pathologically diminished before the patient was treated. Kramer discusses the prospect of “cosmetic psychopharmacology” — when a medication can improve socially desirable traits even in people without pathology, should it be used in such a fashion? Raising the issue should inform, narrowly, psychiatrists’ prescribing practices, and, more broadly, both values-based and fiscally-based societal considerations of antidepressant use.

Prozac

Placebo?

Indeed, antidepressant use has continued to grow wildly in the almost two decades since, and with it the windfall for the drugs’ manufacturers. But you will find very few prescribers, consumers or insurance payors who believe this is the “cosmetic” treatment of those who are not truly ill merely to give them an edge in a competitive society. Instead, the trend has been justified by the redrawing of the boundaries of illness so that a far broader set of conditions are said to be medication-responsive. This is a concern whether you are a naive materialist, who believes in the strictly biological explanation for medication efficacy, or if you attribute the benefits to placebo effect and self-fulfilling prophecy. (Despite the fact that I am a psychopharmacologist, I tend to believe we understand so little about the ‘black box’ of the brain that we are a long way away from being able to tell the difference.)

Kramer worries that newly-skeptical physicians affected by the emperor-has-no-clothes backlash against antidepressant use will fail to treat deserving patients appropriately:

“…It is dangerous for the press to hammer away at the theme that antidepressants are placebos. They’re not. To give the impression that they are is to cause needless suffering…”

He centers his article around a vignette in which a friend of his with post-stroke depression had not been placed on an antidepressant despite the research supporting improved outcome. (Notably, I think, unlike what he did in Listening to Prozac, he is not reflecting on his own prescribing practices, merely those of his colleagues.) But if the meme about antidepressant efficacy changes profoundly enough, some patients will not get better even when they are prescribed these medications, because of the undercutting of the self-fulfilling prophecy. And is that a good thing or a bad thing?

Given that my bias in my work is toward treating sicker patients (I work in a hospital with only a limited outpatient practice with the “worried well”), I am among those who decry the creeping medicalization of everyday life. I don’t know if antidepressants are less “effective” in healthier patients because of the diffuseness and ambiguity about the meaning of “effective”. (Throughout psychiatric research, I see profound confusion and lack of consensus about how to measure outcome.) Severely depressed patients, because core aspects of severe depression include pessimism and despondency, are probably far less susceptible to suggestibility. I don’t know if the research has been done but I would suspect that severe depression sabotages the placebo effect. Thus the observed benefit of antidepressants in this class of patients is more likely to be biological. A nervous system out of whack for some reason can probably be rebalanced better with some pharmacological influence that counteracts the imbalance.

BERLIN - NOVEMBER 22:  Singer and guitarist Br...

Placebo (Brian Molko)

In less ill patients, the balance may indeed shift in favor of placebo effects as the basis for observed benefits. But I have another concern, which has fueled my reluctance to prescribe them too readily, about the expansion of antidepressant use in our society. Although the medications are not, in the formal sense of the term, addictive (i.e. they do not hijack the brain’s craving and pleasure circuitry and there is no tolerance and no acute withdrawal syndrome from abrupt discontinuation of use), I have long worried that too readily prescribing antidepressants for those who do not necessarily start out ‘needing’ them may make them ‘need’ them down the line. Think of it this way. The CNS is a homeostatic mechanism. If it is in balance, it resists and counters changes. (Disease is a perturbation in function outside of the range in which it can by intrinsic mechanisms restore itself to homeostasis.) Give antidepressants to a brain in balance, to amp up certain functions, and counteractive mechanisms may be put into play to restore balance. A new set-point may be established that may persist even after the removal of the medication which was the original influence. Someone who did not need the medication in the first place may be converted into someone who does, perhaps for the rest of their life.

4 comments

  1. HS

    I like your thoughtful consideration of the issues. Science is not advanced by all-or-nothing attitudes and hasty conclusions, though these are obligatory in journalism and politics.

    Like

  2. Aqua, of the Questioners

    there is … no acute withdrawal syndrome from abrupt discontinuation of use

    That’s certainly NOT been my experience, and the packages explicitly say “do not stop taking this medication abruptly”. I agree there’s no psychological cravings, but coming off an anti-depressant can be extremely unpleasant, and so difficult I totally understand why some people give up.

    I wrote about (and photographed) some of my experiences here:
    http://fucking-meds.dreamwidth.org/3983.html
    http://fucking-meds.dreamwidth.org/16395.html
    http://fucking-meds.dreamwidth.org/24532.html
    I hope the swearword in the links isn’t a problem. I’d recommend reading the group to medical professionals – it’s generally very articulate and informed takers of prescription medication discussing their experiences.

    Like

  3. Ray Martin

    Have you looked at the two-part pharma-bashing article in the NYRB bu Marcia Angell? Like to know what you think. Seems, as in so many things, there’s Gaussian distribution of responses to many of these drugs (as there is to placebos). Sometime in the future we’ll have molecular genetic diagnostics to help with patient selection.

    Like

  4. cblacke

    “there is … no acute withdrawal syndrome from abrupt discontinuation of use”

    SSRIs destroyed the self once there. yet without taking them, am soon so suicidal as to be left only to infer a circuit now permanently sabotaged in the CNS itself

    autonomic torment

    and imperative to be taken to keep what’s left alive

    and, 20yrs later, what’s left . . . ? . . . noone nowhere

    Like